閉じる

ComBeNsComputational Behavioral Neuroscience Laboratory

Exploring the principles
of human behavior
based on the information-processing
mechanisms of the brain

Humans are subjects who make decisions and learn while interacting with their environment, and their behavior reflects the characteristics of humans and their environment. To understand humans, we construct behavioral models based on the information-processing mechanisms of the brain and verify them using experimental methods to explore the principles of human behavior and conduct educational research on social applications.

How to join
our lab

As a graduate student
Lab Visit
Lab tours are always welcome. You can find more information in the "Daily Campus Tour." Our laboratory is available for both online and on-site visits.
Lab Experience
If you want to learn more about our research, we also offer "Lab Experience." This is for those who are considering entering our lab. The schedules and content of the Lab Experience are to be announced. We prepare a 1~2 week program consisting of lectures on reinforcement learning models of decision-making and practical training in behavioral experiments.
As a researcher
Now we are looking for a postdoctoral researcher (http://www.naist.jp/en/about_naist/other_information/job_opportunities/230405_2.html) Please contact us if you are interested in this position. We also accept Postdoctoral Fellows (PD) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS). Please contact us for more information.

Research
Procedures

Research
We combine computational approaches using computational models of learning, such as reinforcement learning, and experimental approaches using human subjects.
We also use data-driven approaches using data from large neuroimaging/behavioral databases. Please refer to "Research Topics" below for more detailed information. Neural measurements using EEG and MRI are conducted at ATR Brain Information and Communication Laboratories. ATR is located about 15 minutes by car from NAIST.
Surpervise
First, students are required to read previous studies on the research theme of their interest and take time to decide on a research theme. Once the theme is decided, we will surpervise to plan experiments, analyze data, summarize the results, and present the results. Especially in the case of research themes involving neuroimaging experiments, we will spend time on training experimental techniques and data analysis methods.
Organization
We collaborate with Mathematical Informatics Lab (Prof. Kazushi Ikeda). Lab seminars are held jointly. Associate Prof. Tanaka is also the head of the Department of Neural Computation in Decision-Making (NCD) at ATR Brain Information and Communication Research Laboratories. Our lab works closely with the members of ATR.

Research Topics

  1. 1.Neural mechanisms of delay discounting

    Reinforcement learning, a theoretical framework in which optimal behavior is learned by trial and error through interaction with the environment, has been proposed as a computational model of decision-making in animals, including humans. To elucidate the neural mechanisms involved in reward prediction, we conducted fMRI, a non-invasive functional brain imaging technique, on healthy participants and analyzed behavioral and brain activity data in a Markov decision problem for long-term and short-term reward prediction using a reinforcement learning model. The results revealed that the ventral part of the cortico-striatal circuit is involved in the prediction with a large discount rate, and the dorsal part is in the prediction with a small discount rate, indicating that the brain performs reward prediction with different discount rates in parallel.

    Tanaka, S. C., Doya, K., Okada, G., Ueda, K., Okamoto, Y., & Yamawaki, S. (2004). Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nature neuroscience, 7(8), 887–893. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1279

  2. 2.Delay discounting and serotonin

    Previous studies have reported that serotonin, a brain substance, is involved in time discounting, mainly in animal experiments using rats, but consistent results have yet to be obtained in humans. To investigate the relationship between discount rates and serotonin, we developed an intertemporal choice task in which participants choose between a small immediate reward and a large, time-consuming reward and measured and analyzed their behaviors and brain activities during the task while artificially adjusting the level of serotonin in their brains. The results showed that when serotonin levels were normal, multiple discount rates of reward prediction were distributed from ventral to dorsal regions of the striatum, which is consistent with previous studies. On the other hand, we found that the ventral part of the striatum was predominant when serotonin was low, and the dorsal part of the striatum was predominant when serotonin was high, indicating that the activity of the striatum is affected by the action of serotonin.

    Tanaka, S. C., Schweighofer, N., Asahi, S., Shishida, K., Okamoto, Y., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K. (2007). Serotonin differentially regulates short- and long-term prediction of rewards in the ventral and dorsal striatum. PloS one, 2(12), e1333 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001333

  3. 3.Altered delay discount and problematic behavior

    Delay discounting is a topic of economics study and has recently been pointed out to be related to social and health problems such as multiple debts and obesity. It has also been used as a model of impulsivity, a symptom of psychiatric disorders, and a behavioral trait of adolescents. Therefore, we clarify the relationship between the parameters of the decision-making model and problematic behaviors and contribute to the understanding and preventing social and health problems. In response to an economic study showing that people with a high discount rate and no asymmetry in the discount rate between rewards and losses (sign effect) have higher rates of multiple debts and obesity, we conducted an experiment to elucidate the brain mechanism of the sign effect. In an experiment using the intertemporal choice between rewards and losses task, we estimated the discount rate from the subjects' behavioral data and compared the brain activities of the groups with and without the sign effect (asymmetry of the time discount rate between rewards and losses in behavior). The results suggest that the asymmetry of reward and loss in striatal activity is caused by the sign effect, i.e., the asymmetry of reward and loss in behavior.

    Tanaka, S. C., Yamada, K., Yoneda, H., & Ohtake, F. (2014). Neural mechanisms of gain-loss asymmetry in temporal discounting. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(16), 5595–5602. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5169-12.2014

  4. 4.Temporal credit assignment and serotonin

    The impulsive choice to frequently select "small immediate rewards" in intertemporal choice occurs when the weight of delayed rewards is reduced when the discount rate is high, but it can also occur when delayed rewards obtained in learning are only associated with the immediate past (credit assignment problem). To test this, we estimated the decay rate of subjects' temporal credit assignment from their behaviors in a learning task in which there is a temporal delay between the past action and the current reward under the serotonin level is manipulated. We found that the decay rate of the temporal credit assignment to the loss was larger when the serotonin level was decreased than in the normal condition. These results suggest that serotonin is involved in both the future time scale (discount rate of reward prediction) and the past time scale (decay rate of temporal credit assignment).

    Tanaka, S. C., Shishida, K., Schweighofer, N., Okamoto, Y., Yamawaki, S., & Doya, K. (2009). Serotonin affects association of aversive outcomes to past actions. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(50), 15669–15674. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2799-09.2009

  5. 5.Data-driven approach

    Computational psychiatry has gained momentum, in which the mechanisms of psychiatry disorders are understood as computational models by applying the hypothesis-testing approach to patients with psychiatry disorders, as described in topics 3 and 4 above. On the other hand, the data-driven approach, which combines multivariate brain and behavioral data with machine learning, is attempting to understand the mechanisms of psychiatry disorders and individual characteristics in a "holistic" manner without being bound by any specific hypothesis. Examples of such approaches include identifying disorders using resting-state functional connectivity patterns, biotype identification, and prediction of individual characteristics. However, data-driven analysis with high accuracy and generalization performance requires a large amount of data and the development of algorithms to extract features successfully. The applicant has elucidated the brain network common to various types of anxiety by using large data sets and practical machine learning algorithms tailored to the characteristics of each data set. By extracting features from brain activity data reflecting different types of anxiety, i.e., brain activity during anxiety-provoking tasks and resting-state brain activity related to daily anxiety characteristics, and reducing the dimension of the features, we identified the brain network common to these types of anxiety. We developed a method for identifying patients with anxiety-related diseases from this brain network. We confirmed that it is possible to discriminate patients with anxiety-related disorders from this brain network.

    Takagi, Y., Sakai, Y., Abe, Y., Nishida, S., Harrison, B. J., Martínez-Zalacaín, I., Soriano-Mas, C., Narumoto, J., & Tanaka, S. C. (2018). A common brain network among state, trait, and pathological anxiety from whole-brain functional connectivity. NeuroImage, 172, 506–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.080

  6. 6.Computational model of obsessive-compulsive disorder

    We use computational models to describe human behavior and investigate the relationship between the parameters, individual characteristics, and neural mechanisms. Specifically, we are constructing computational models of psychiatric disorders and validating them with empirical data. We model obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) symptoms as implicitly learned maladaptive behaviors. Simulations in the reinforcement learning framework show that agents implicitly learn to respond to intrusive thoughts when the memory trace signal for past actions decays differently for positive and negative prediction errors. Moreover, this model extends our understanding of the therapeutic effects of behavioral therapy in OCD. Using empirical data, we confirm that patients with OCD show extremely imbalanced traces, which are normalized by serotonin enhancers. We find that healthy participants also vary in their obsessive-compulsive tendencies, consistent with the degree of imbalanced traces. These behavioral characteristics can be generalized to variations in the healthy population beyond the spectrum of clinical phenotypes.

    Sakai, Y., Sakai, Y., Abe, Y., Narumoto, J., & Tanaka, S. C. (2022). Memory trace imbalance in reinforcement and punishment systems can reinforce implicit choices leading to obsessive-compulsive behavior. Cell reports, 40(9), 111275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111275

  7. 7.Large-scale neuroimaging database

    Recently, the reproducibility of results of scientific research has become a major issue in scientific fields as a whole, and various attempts have been made to ensure reproducibility in brain research. Among them, large-scale data collection and sharing are considered the most important to ensure sufficient statistical power. As a member of the multi-institutional human brain MRI large-scale database project, we have constructed a globally valuable multicenter and multi-disease brain MRI database that is publicly available.

    Tanaka, S. C., Yamashita, A., Yahata, N., Itahashi, T., Lisi, G., Yamada, T., Ichikawa, N., Takamura, M., Yoshihara, Y., Kunimatsu, A., Okada, N., Hashimoto, R., Okada, G., Sakai, Y., Morimoto, J., Narumoto, J., Shimada, Y., Mano, H., Yoshida, W., Seymour, B., … Imamizu, H. (2021). A multi-site, multi-disorder resting-state magnetic resonance image database. Scientific data, 8(1), 227. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-021-01004-8

Member

Associate Professor (tenure track)

Saori C. TANAKA
田中 沙織 Saori C. TANAKA
Link
ResearchmapGoogle Scholar

特任助教

荻島 大凱 Hiroyoshi OGISHIMA
荻島 大凱 Hiroyoshi OGISHIMA
Link
Researchmap
Wendyam Eric Lionel Ilboudo
Wendyam Eric Lionel Ilboudo
Link
Google Scholar

客員助教


川島 一朔
Issaku KAWASHIMA, ATR

委託教員


酒井 雄希
Yuki SAKAI, ATR

学生

佐野 海士

佐野 海士
Kaito SANO
D1

Gao Yulun

Gao YulunD1


中川 綾子
Ayako NAKAGAWA
M2


大西 達也
Tatsuya ONISHI
M1


白川 琢磨
Takuma SHIRAKAWA
M1


成田 大祐
Daisuke NARITA
M1


西谷 香紀
Kano NISHITANI
M1


児玉 創太郎
Sotaro KODAMA
M1


渡邉 奈緒美
Naomi WATANABE
M1

Alumni


Erik HAINZLMEIER
Technical University of Munich


小池 教
Minori KOIKE

Publications

  1. Journal

    Please referrer to Researchmap.
    https://researchmap.jp/xsaori/published_papers

Contact

Please contact us from here -> Google form will open

Location:
Computational Behavioral Neuroscience Laboratory Division of Information Science
Nara Institute of Science and Technology (6th floor, Building B, Division of Information Science)

NAIST 情報科学領域